The Kernel of Lies: Investigating Stereotype Threat with Fake Stereotypes
2017
- 35Usage
Metric Options: CountsSelecting the 1-year or 3-year option will change the metrics count to percentiles, illustrating how an article or review compares to other articles or reviews within the selected time period in the same journal. Selecting the 1-year option compares the metrics against other articles/reviews that were also published in the same calendar year. Selecting the 3-year option compares the metrics against other articles/reviews that were also published in the same calendar year plus the two years prior.
Example: if you select the 1-year option for an article published in 2019 and a metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019. If you select the 3-year option for the same article published in 2019 and the metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019, 2018 and 2017.
Citation Benchmarking is provided by Scopus and SciVal and is different from the metrics context provided by PlumX Metrics.
Example: if you select the 1-year option for an article published in 2019 and a metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019. If you select the 3-year option for the same article published in 2019 and the metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019, 2018 and 2017.
Citation Benchmarking is provided by Scopus and SciVal and is different from the metrics context provided by PlumX Metrics.
Metrics Details
- Usage35
- Abstract Views35
Poster Description
Previous research examining stereotype threat has shown that individuals who are reminded of a stereotype pertaining to their social group are more likely to conform to the stereotype (Steele, 1995). The idea of “stigma consciousness” arose when researchers wanted to see if individuals feared they would be stereotyped by others once their group membership became salient (Pinel, 1999). In other words, reading or hearing a stereotype about one's in-group could influence individuals to worry that they personally are being included in the group and thus, a target of the stereotype. Currently, there is no known research regarding stereotype threat evoked with the use of fake stereotypes. That is, will a person fulfill a stereotype, if the stereotype is fake. To test this, participants were invited to participate in a study regarding political affiliation. Participants were asked to identify their political affiliation so they could read information about their in-group. Regardless of their actual political affiliation, participants were randomly assigned to read a list of stereotypes targeting their group that included statements that were either positive (e.g., less racist), negative (e.g., more racist), or irrelevant (e.g., taller) to the current dependent measure. Then, they completed a survey packet that contained multiple measures, including a measure of mental health stigma (Community Attitudes Toward the Mentally Ill; CAMI; Taylor & Dear, 1981), empathy (the interpersonal reactivity index; Davis, 1980), and racism (modern racism; McConahay, 1986). It was hypothesized that participants scores would mimic their condition, regardless of valence. Data is currently being collected.
Bibliographic Details
Provide Feedback
Have ideas for a new metric? Would you like to see something else here?Let us know