Predicting the Flynn Effect through word abstractness : Results from the National Intelligence Tests support Flynn's explanation
Intelligence, ISSN: 0160-2896, Vol: 57, Page: 7-14
2016
- 3Citations
- 22Captures
Metric Options: CountsSelecting the 1-year or 3-year option will change the metrics count to percentiles, illustrating how an article or review compares to other articles or reviews within the selected time period in the same journal. Selecting the 1-year option compares the metrics against other articles/reviews that were also published in the same calendar year. Selecting the 3-year option compares the metrics against other articles/reviews that were also published in the same calendar year plus the two years prior.
Example: if you select the 1-year option for an article published in 2019 and a metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019. If you select the 3-year option for the same article published in 2019 and the metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019, 2018 and 2017.
Citation Benchmarking is provided by Scopus and SciVal and is different from the metrics context provided by PlumX Metrics.
Example: if you select the 1-year option for an article published in 2019 and a metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019. If you select the 3-year option for the same article published in 2019 and the metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019, 2018 and 2017.
Citation Benchmarking is provided by Scopus and SciVal and is different from the metrics context provided by PlumX Metrics.
Article Description
The current study investigates the Flynn Effect (FE) and its relation to abstract thinking ability. We compare two cohorts of Estonian students (1933/36, n = 888; 2006, n = 912) using the Concepts (Logical Selection) subtest of the Estonian adaptation of the National Intelligence Tests (NIT). The item presentation order of the subtest correlates with the abstractness of the words used in the items (r =.609) of the subtest. The different test results (right, wrong and missing answers) were analysed in order to make an estimate of the FE magnitude. The FE for abstract thinking ability of those samples was 1.06 Hedges' g (adjusted for guessing). The magnitude of the FE is dependent upon the degree of difficulty of the items (an item's difficulty is estimated by determining its abstractness and its familiarity to students). The more difficult part of the subtest (the second half) showed a FE = 1.80 whereas the easier part (the first half) of the subtest showed a FE =.72. Word abstractness was a strong predictor of all the testing results in both cohorts (Beta =.700). The familiarity of words used in the test items has no correlation with the test results if word abstractness is controlled in both cohorts. Our findings support Flynn's explanation that the FE is primarily an indicator of the rise in abstract thinking ability.
Bibliographic Details
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289616300253; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.03.003; http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?partnerID=HzOxMe3b&scp=84962262872&origin=inward; https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0160289616300253; https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.03.003
Elsevier BV
Provide Feedback
Have ideas for a new metric? Would you like to see something else here?Let us know