Affective and non-affective psycholinguistic norms for 500 Chinese three-character idiomatic expressions
Behavior Research Methods, ISSN: 1554-3528, Vol: 57, Issue: 4, Page: 116
2025
Metric Options: CountsSelecting the 1-year or 3-year option will change the metrics count to percentiles, illustrating how an article or review compares to other articles or reviews within the selected time period in the same journal. Selecting the 1-year option compares the metrics against other articles/reviews that were also published in the same calendar year. Selecting the 3-year option compares the metrics against other articles/reviews that were also published in the same calendar year plus the two years prior.
Example: if you select the 1-year option for an article published in 2019 and a metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019. If you select the 3-year option for the same article published in 2019 and the metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019, 2018 and 2017.
Citation Benchmarking is provided by Scopus and SciVal and is different from the metrics context provided by PlumX Metrics.
Example: if you select the 1-year option for an article published in 2019 and a metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019. If you select the 3-year option for the same article published in 2019 and the metric category shows 90%, that means that the article or review is performing better than 90% of the other articles/reviews published in that journal in 2019, 2018 and 2017.
Citation Benchmarking is provided by Scopus and SciVal and is different from the metrics context provided by PlumX Metrics.
Article Description
Multiword expressions, especially idiomatic expressions, convey rich emotional information. To explore the relationship between affective and non-affective psycholinguistic dimensions in the processing of idiomatic expressions, we collected normative data for two affective (i.e., valence and arousal) and four non-affective variables (i.e., familiarity, transparency, ambiguity, age of acquisition) for 500 Chinese three-character idiomatic expressions, based on responses from 418 native Chinese speakers. Our results showed a quadratic U-shaped relationship between valence and arousal, with neutral idiomatic expressions rated as less arousing than negative or positive ones. Furthermore, more transparent idiomatic expressions were more strongly valenced (either negative or positive) and more arousing. Idiomatic expressions more often used figuratively evoked more negative or stronger emotional responses. In addition, idiomatic expressions with greater negativity were acquired earlier but occurred less frequently. Regarding the non-affective variables, more transparent idiomatic expressions were perceived as more familiar and acquired earlier. Idiomatic expressions more often used figuratively, acquired earlier, or occurred more frequently were rated as more familiar. Lastly, idiomatic expressions that were more often used literally were more transparent and frequent. This study provides a large-scale database for examining the impact of affective and non-affective psycholinguistic factors on Chinese idiomatic expression processing, and it sheds light on the interrelated cognitive mechanisms underlying figurative language processing and emotion.
Bibliographic Details
Springer Science and Business Media LLC
Provide Feedback
Have ideas for a new metric? Would you like to see something else here?Let us know